Syllabus

Behavioral Pharmacology (PG 660) Fall, 1998

Course Title: Behavioral Pharmacology (PG 660)

Instructor: Christopher Newland, Ph.D.

Department of Psychology.

Room 110, Thach Hall. newlamc@mail.auburn.edu 844-6479

Meeting Times: Mondays: 12:00-2:00

Wednesdays: 1:00-3:00

Room: Thach 210.

Text: Julien, R. M. (1995) A Primer of Drug Action. New York: W. H. Freeman. (or similar book)

Selected readings to be made available.

Papers from the primary literature are listed at the end of the syllabus.

Behavioral pharmacology is a discipline that straddles experimental psychology (and particularly the experimental analysis of behavior) and pharmacology. Many date it to a paper by Peter B. Dews in the Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics in 1955, which showed that the behavioral effects of pentobarbital and amphetamine depended upon the schedule of reinforcement maintaining that behavior but its roots go back much further. The discipline differs from others involving drugs that act upon the nervous system in that behavioral pharmacologists emphasize the study of the behavior of intact organisms (rather than of tissue samples) and the behavioral mechanisms of drug action are studied as painstakingly as are the pharmacological mechanisms of action. The abused drug, cocaine, offers an example. This drug may be studied as a catecholamine agonist, a pharmacological mechanism, and its interaction with that class of receptors may be the target of such investigations. Cocaine also acts as a positive reinforcer and its reinforcing properties may be assessed as a function of the availability of alternative reinforcers, the schedule of delivery, response effort, or the degree to which delivery is contingent upon responding. These are all behavioral mechanisms. What is learned about cocaine as a catecholamine agonist applies to other drugs that act on that receptor. What is learned about cocaine as a positive reinforcer applies to other drugs whose response-contingent delivery maintains behavior, regardless of the receptor.

The experimental analysis of behavior has contributed both a new theoretical framework for the study of drug-behavior interactions, and powerful new techniques for understanding drug actions. These techniques and the conceptual framework surrounding them will form the basis for our studies. The characterization of drug classes on the basis of their effects on operant behavior is a widely recognized and well-used technique in both academic and industrial pharmacology. These techniques enable fine-grained characterizations of drug action, the use of drugs to uncover brain-behavior relationships, the early detection of drugs with potential for abuse, basic research on drug taking, and behavioral mechanisms by which drugs used in clinical settings are effective. We will see many replications of Dews' original discovery that the conditions under which drugs are administered are powerful determinants of their behavioral effects.

Course Structure. The course will be structured as a seminar based upon the primary literature. I have selected papers representing conceptual or methodological approaches to the more fundamental drug-behavior interactions in human and nonhuman species. We cover discriminative and reinforcing effects of drugs, interactions of drugs and the events maintaining behavior (e.g., special properties of behavior under aversive control), behavioral tolerance, drug effects on motor function, discrimination, learning, and memory. As befits a graduate-level seminar, we will spend a lot of time on the interrelationships between our conceptual understanding of phenomena and the methods used to discover them.

Paper presentations. The first portion of the course is divided into sections representing different areas. During the last class of a previous topic, or the first class of a new topic, the instructor will provide an introductory lecture of about 20-60 min. Students will then present papers relevant to the topic under discussion in the remaining class time. We will try to get through about 3-5 papers from the primary literature every week. The papers that we will cover are listed in bold after the schedule and are available in the course packet at Speedy Print. These are required readings. One student will be assigned to present one of the papers but the other students will be prepared to discuss the paper. We will be able to spend 10-40 min on each paper, depending upon the complexity of the paper. Some may take more time. Presenters should use the board, prepare a handout summarizing the paper, have transparencies of the important figures and your summaries. An overhead projector will be available. To summarize: each student will be responsible for reading and discussing all the papers listed in bold but only one student will present it.

In your discussion be thorough, concise, and clear. Try the "tell'em" strategy: "tell 'em what your going to tell 'em, tell 'em, and tell 'em what you told 'em." Be sure to set up each paper by summarizing what question is being addressed, why it is an important question, and what methods might be used to address it. When discussing drugs always use the generic name, state what class they belong to, its half-life, and what neurotransmitter(s) is (are) thought to underlie the drug's action when this is known. Only papers using common drugs were selected, so the information will be available in Julien or some similar book. It is the presenter's responsibility to provide the background material required to understand the paper, and this may mean going to the library to read papers referred to prominently. Keep all discussion focused on what was done, what happened, and how this was interpreted. Describe all procedures carefully, and in terms of what was done and what happened. Always place all behavior in the context of the environmental contingencies such as three-term contingency of reinforcement or the contingencies of respondent conditioning. This may be difficult at first, but it is worth the effort. We will not tolerate folksy descriptions of behavior, but instead, will respect the spirit of Lloyd Morgan's canon:

In no case may we interpret an action as the outcome of the exercise of a higher psychical faculty, if it can be interpreted as the outcome of the exercise of one which stands lower in the psychological scale. (From Morgan, C.L. (1894), An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. London: Walter Scott.)

When presenting and evaluating the papers, consider the following points:

-- Are dose-effect data presented?

-- Are the data relevant to the performance of individual organisms?

-- What is the pharmacological relevance of the paper? What does it tell us about the action of the drugs used?

-- What is the behavioral significance? What does it tell us about behavior?

-- What is the behavioral mechanism of the drug's action?

-- Where does the drug's action fit into the three-term contingency of reinforcement? Is it a stimulus? a consequence?

-- Is the discussion well-grounded in data?

-- Are adequate behavioral controls applied? Is there an alternative behavioral interpretation?

-- Are adequate pharmacological controls applied? Vehicle injections? Drugs from different categories?

-- Draw from the behavioral mechanisms and describe how the results might apply to human behavior.

-- Does this paper attempt heterogeneous or homogeneous reduction?





Evaluation. Evaluation will be based upon your presentations, class participation (quality and quantity), and two take-home exams. By 9:00 a.m. on the morning of each class, I want you to e-mail me at least one question about the readings for that day. The question should reflect a careful reading of the materials. I use these questions to guide how material is covered, and what is said about it. The distribution will be as follows: Each presentation will count as a single unit, the exams as three units each, participation (including the questions) counts as one unit. Your course grade will be a simple average of these.

Following are some of the criteria used to evaluate the presentations:

-- Clear description of the research question and coverage of the points listed above.

-- Clear description of the methods. (important details, not all details).

-- Graphical presentation of the results. This can be a transparency, or on the blackboard.

-- Presentation of the author's conclusions.

-- The extent to which you go beyond the paper and incorporate what you know, or what you are learning in this course. This can be in the form of critical comment on weaknesses, unanswered questions raised, further research prompted by this experiment, or extensions to understanding human behavior.

Following are some of the criteria for evaluating participation of those not presenting:

-- Clear evidence that you have read the paper.

-- Questions asked and points of discussion raised.

-- Insights about how two or more of the papers tie together (especially relevant for those with no responsibilities to present during a class).

-- Participation in discussion.
Schedule of Activities
Wk Class Date Topic Readings
General Issues.
1 1 23 Sep Course Introduction. Review of basic behavior and pharmacology. Julien
2 2 28 Sep History, mechanisms, and reductionism. [1-3]
Drugs as Participants in Behavioral Processes
3 30 Sep Drugs as reinforcers: General issues [4-6]
3 4 5 Oct Drugs as reinforcers: Primary readings. [7-13]
5 7 Oct Drugs as reinforcers: Primary readings.

Drugs as discriminative stimuli: general issues.

[14, 15]
4 6 12 Oct Drugs as discriminative stimuli and subjective effects of drugs. Primary readings. [16-20]
7 14 Oct Drugs as discriminative stimuli: Primary readings.

Drugs as conditional and unconditional stimuli: general issues.

[21]
5 8 19 Oct Drugs as conditional and unconditional stimuli: primary readings. [22-27]
9 21 Oct Behavior maintained by positive reinforcers: general issues.

Behavior maintained by positive reinforcers: Primary readings.

[28]
Drug Effects on Behavior.
6 10 26 Oct Behavioral history: Primary readings.

Behavior under aversive control: General issues.

[29-36]
11 28 Oct Behavior under aversive control: Primary readings. [37-42]
7 12 2 Nov Behavior under aversive control: Primary readings.

Motor function: general issues.

13 4 Nov Motor function: primary readings. [43-46]
8 14 9 Nov Drug effects and contextual control over behavior: general issues [47-53]
15 11 Nov Drug effects on contextual control: primary readings.
9 16 16 Nov Drug effects on contextual control: primary readings.
17 18 Nov Drug effects on contextual control: primary readings.
10 18 30 Nov Behavioral mechanisms of tolerance: General issues. [54-59]
19 2 Dec Behavioral mechanisms of tolerance: Primary readings.
20 10 Dec Final Exam day. [60-63]




References

1. Marr, M.J., Behavioral pharmacology: issues of reductionism and causality., in Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology. 1990, Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ. p. 1-12.

2. Laties, V.G., Lessons from the history of behavioral pharmacology, in Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology: Developmental Behavioral Pharmacology. 1986, L. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ. p. 21-39.

3. Pickens, R., Behavioral Pharmacology: A brief history, in Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology. 1977, Academic Press: New York. p. 229-257.

4. Stolerman, I., Drugs of abuse: behavioural principles, methods and terms. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 1992. 13: p. 170-176.

5. Hursh, S.R. and G. Winger, Normalized demand for drugs and other reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1995. 64: p. 373-384.

6. Young, A. and S. Herling, Drugs as reinforcers: Studies in laboratory animals., in Behavioral Analysis of Drug Dependence. 1986, Academic Press: New York. p. 9-68.

7. Meisch, R.A. and R. Spiga, Matching under nonindependent variable-ratio schedules of drug reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1998. 70: p. 23-34.

8. Holter, S.M., M. Engelmann, C. Kirschke, G. Liebsch, L. Langraf, and R. Spanagel, Long-term ethanol self-adminstration with repeated ethanol deprivation episodes changes ethanol drinking pattern and increases anxiety-related behaviour during ethanol deprivation in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1998. 9: p. 41-48.

9. Caine, S.B. and G.F. Koob, Effects of dopamine D-1 and D-2 antagonists on cocaine self-administration under different schedules of reinforcement in the rat. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1994. 270: p. 209-218.

10. Carroll, M.E., G.G. Carmona, and S.A. May, Modifying drug-reinforced behavior by altering the economic conditions of the drug and a non-drug reinforcer. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1991. 56: p. 361-376.

11. Comer, S.D., E.D. collins, and M.W. Fischman, Choice between money and intranasal heroin in morphine-maintained humans. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1997. 8: p. 677-690.

12. English, J.A., J.K. Bowlett, and W.L. Woolverton, Unit-price analysis of opiod consumption by monkeys responding under a progressive ratio schedule of drug-injection. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1995. 64: p. 361-371.

13. Foltin, R.W., Food and amphetamine self-administration by baboons: effects of alternatives. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1997. 68: p. 47-66.

14. Stolerman, I.P., Drug discrimination, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 217-243.

15. Preston, K.L. and G.E. Bigelow, Subjective and discriminative effects of drugs. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1991. 2: p. 293-313.

16. Rees, D.C. and R.L. Balster, Attenuation of the discriminative stimulus properties of ethanol and oxazepam, but not of pentobarbital, by RO 15-4513 in mice. J Pharmacol Exp Therap, 1988. 244: p. 592-598.

17. Massey, B.W., D.E. McMillan, and W.D. Wessinger, Discriminative-stimulus control by morphine in the pigeon under a fixed- interval schedule of reinforcement. Behavoral Pharmacol., 1992. 3: p. 475-488.

18. Gauvin, D.V. and A.M. Young, Evidence for perceptual masking of the discriminative morphine stimulus. Psychopharmacology, 1989. 98: p. 212-221.

19. Preston, K.L., G.E. Bigelow, W.K. Bickel, and I.A. Liebson, Drug discrimination in human postaddicts: Agonist-antagonist opiods. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1989. 250: p. 184-196.

20. DeGrandpre, R.J., W.K. Bickel, and S.T. Higgins, Emergent equivalence relations between interoceptive (drug) and exteroceptive (visual) stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1992. 58: p. 9-18.

21. Cunningham, C.L., Pavlovian drug conditioning, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 349-380.

22. Newlin, D.B., Conditioned compensatory response to alcohol placebo in humans. Psychopharmacology, 1986. 88: p. 247-251.

23. Panlilio, L.V. and C.W. Schindler, Conditioned locomotor-activating and reinforcing effects of discrete stimuli paired with i.p. cocaine. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1997. 8: p. 691-698.

24. Goldberg, S.R. and C.R. Schuster, Conditioned suppression by a stimulus associated with nalorphine in morphine dependent monkeys. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1967. 10: p. 235-242.

25. Siegel, S., Morphine analgesic tolerance: its situation specificity supports a Pavlovian conditioning model. Science, 1976. 193: p. 323-325.

26. Siegel, S., Evidence from rats that morphine tolerance is a learned response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology., 1978. 89: p. 498-506.

27. Ternes, J.W., C.P. O'Brien, and T.T. Testa, Rapid acquisition of conditioned responses to hydromorphone in detoxified heroin addicts. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 1982. 18: p. 215-219.

28. van Haaren, F., Schedule-controlled behavior: positive reinforcement, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 81-99.

29. Bizot, J.C., Effects of psychoactive drugs on temporal discrimination in rats. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1997. 8: p. 293-308.

30. Howell, L.L., L.D. Byrd, and M.J. Marr, Similarities in the rate-altering effects of white noise and cocaine. J Exp Anal Behav, 1986. 46: p. 381-394.

31. Branch, M.N. and L.R. Gollub, A detailed analysis of the effects of d-amphetamine on behavior under fixed- interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior., 1974. 23: p. 519-540.

32. Higgins, S.T. and M.L. Stitzer, Time allocation in a concurrent schedule of social interaction and monetary reinforcement: Effects of d-amphetamine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 1988. 31: p. 227-231.

33. Barrett, J.E., Behavioral history as a determinant of the effects of d-amphetamine on punished behavior. Science, 1977. 198: p. 67-69.

34. Terrace, H.S., Errorless discrimination learning in the pigeon: effects of chlorpromazine and imipramine. Science, 1966. 140: p. 318-319.

35. Urbain, C., A. Poling, J. Millam, and T. Thompson, d-amphetamine and fixed-interval performance: effects of operant history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1978. 29: p. 385-392.

36. Heyman, G.M. and L.S. Seiden, A parametric description of amphetamine's effect on response rate: changes in reinforcement efficacy and response topography. Psychopharmacology, 1985. 85: p. 154-161.

37. Dworkin, S.I., R.C. Pitts, and M. Galizio, Schedule-controlled behavior: negative reinforcement, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 101-116.

38. Spealman, R.D., Comparison of drug effects on responding punished by pressurized air or electroc shock delivery in squirrel monkeys: pentobarbital, chlordiazepoxide, d-amphetamine, and cocaine. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1979. 209: p. 309-315.

39. Wood, R.W., P.H. Warren, and B. Weiss, Attenuated aversiveness of electric shock during nitrous oxide exposure. J Pharmacol Exp Therap, 1980. 213: p. 128-132.

40. Barrett, J.E., L.S. Brady, and J.M. Witkin, Behavioral studies with anxiolytic drugs. I. Interactions of the benzodiazepine antagonist RO 15-1788 with chlordiazepoxide, pentobarbital and ethanol. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1985. 233: p. 554-559.

41. Downs, D.A. and J.H. Woods, Fixed-ratio escape and avoidance-escape from naltrexone in morphine-dependent monkeys: effects of naloxone dose and morphine pretreatment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1975. 23: p. 415-427.

42. Galizio, M. and M. Perone, Variable-interval schedules if timeout from avoidance: effects of chlordiazeposice, CGS 8216, morphine, and naltrexone. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1987. 47: p. 115-126.

43. Newland, M.C., Operant behavior and the measurement of motor dysfunction, in Neurobehavioral Toxicity: analysis and interpretation. 1994, Raven Press: New York. p. 273-297.

44. Heyman, G.M., M.M. Monaghan, and D.E. Clody, Low doses of cis-flupentixol attenuate motor performance. Psychopharmacology, 1987. 93: p. 477-482.

45. Tang, M., C.E. Lau, and J.L. Falk, Midazolam and discriminative motor control: chronic administration, withdrawal, and modulation by the antagonist Ro15-1788. J Pharmacol Exp Therap, 1988. 246: p. 1053-1060.

46. Fowler, S.C., S.E. Bowen, and M.J. Kallman, Practice-augmented tolerance to triazolam: evidence from an anlaysis of operant response durations and interresponse times. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1993. 4: p. 147-157.

47. Schindler, C.W., Classical conditioning, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 53-79.

48. Picker, M.J. and S.S. Negus, Drugs and stimulus control: generalization, discrimination, and threshold procedures, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 117-145.

49. Laties, V.G. and B. Weiss, Influence of drugs on behavior controlled by internal and external stimuli. J Pharmacol Exp Therap, 1966. 152: p. 388-396.

50. Wenger, G.R. and D.W. Wright, Disruption of performance under a titrating matching-to-sample schedule of reinforcement by drugs of abuse. J Pharmacol Exp Therap, 1990. 254: p. 258-269.

51. Thompson, D.M. and J.M. Moerschbaecher, An experimental analysis of the effects of d-amphetamine and cocaine on the acquisition and performance of response chains in monkeys. J Exp Anal Behav, 1979. 32: p. 433-444.

52. Baron, S.P. and J.M. Moerschbaecher, Disruption of learning by excitatory amino acid receptor antagonists. Behavioural Pharmacology, 1996. 6: p. 573-584.

53. Bickel, W.K., S.T. Higgins, and J.R. Hughes, The effects of diazepam and triazolam on repeated acquisition and performance of response sequences with an observing response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1991. 56: p. 217-237.

54. Branch, M.N., Behavioral factors in drug tolerance, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 329-347.

55. Woolverton, W.L., D. Kandel, and C.R. Schuster, Tolerance and cross-tolerance to cocaine and d-amphetamine. J Pharmacol Exp Therap, 1978. 205: p. 525-535.

56. Schuster, C.R., W.S. Dockens, and J.H. Woods, Behavioral variables affecting the development of amphetamine tolerance. Psychopharmacol., 1966. 9: p. 170-182.

57. Holloway, F.A., D.A. King, R.C. Michaelis, R.D. Harland, and D.C. Bird, Tolerance to ethanol's disruptive effects on operant behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology, 1989. 99: p. 479-485.

58. Rees, D.C., R.W. Wood, and V.G. Laties, Stimulus control and the development of behavioral tolerance to daily injections of d-amphetamine in the rat. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1987. 240: p. 65-73.

59. Smith, J.B., Situational specificity of tolerance to decreased operant resonding by morphine and l-nantradol. Psychopharmacology, 1991. 103: p. 115-120.

60. Higgins, S.T., W.K. Bickel, and J.R. Hughes, Methods in the human behavioral pharmacology of drug abuse, in Methods in Behavioral Pharmacology, F. van Haaren, Editor. 1993, Elsevier: New York. p. 475-497.

61. Higgins, S.T., A.J. Budney, W.K. Bickel, J.R. Hughes, F. Foerg, and G. Badger, Achieving cocaine abstinence with a behavioral approach. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1993. 150: p. 763-769.

62. Higgins, S.t., A.J. Budney, W.K. Bickel, G.J. Badger, F.E. Foerg, and D. Ogden, Outpatient behavioral treatment for cocaine dependence: one year outcome. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 1995. 3: p. 205-212.

63. Silverman, K., S.T. Higgins, R.K. Brooner, I.D. Montoya, E.J. Cone, C.R. Schuster, and K.L. Preston, Sustained cocaine abstinence in methadone maintenance patients through voucher-based reinforcement therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1996. 53: p. 409-415.

 

Send mail to newlamc@auburn.edu with questions or comments about this web site.
Last modified: July 08, 2010